dc.contributor.author | Badio, Janusz | |
dc.contributor.editor | Badio, Janusz | |
dc.date.accessioned | 2021-02-01T10:19:12Z | |
dc.date.available | 2021-02-01T10:19:12Z | |
dc.date.issued | 2020 | |
dc.identifier.citation | Badio, J. (2020). Use of stereotypical social gender roles in an L2 narrative construal task by advanced, Polish learners of English. In. J. Badio (Ed.),Focus on events and narratives in language, psychology, social and medical practice, (pp. 279-39). Łódź: WUŁ, http://dx.doi.org/10.18778/8220-051-5.03 | pl_PL |
dc.identifier.isbn | 978-83-8220-051-5 | |
dc.identifier.uri | http://hdl.handle.net/11089/33249 | |
dc.description.abstract | The article presents a study of the process of comprehension of an input joke and its subsequent retelling in writing by Polish advanced students (n=36) of English as a foreign language. The research question was whether people are guided by stereotypical social gender roles and cultural schemata during comprehension and production of a joke involving events that make up a story. The joke included references to female (wife) and male (husband) participants as well as a secretary (gender not specified), names Gloria and Mike and a number of objects such as: violets, chocolates, handbag or laptop. It was predicted that the VM version of the joke (the version with the name Mike, supposedly male secretary) would take the participants more time to interpret as compared to the VF version (with Gloria as a most possible candidate for the secretary role). This hypothesis was indeed confirmed, but the experimental subjects did not take significantly more time to write their versions (VM and VF conditions) of the story. The qualitative analysis demonstrated variable cultural bias during the construction of the participants’ roles and relations, often against or in the absence of clear instructions in the input to do so. | pl_PL |
dc.language.iso | en | pl_PL |
dc.publisher | Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Łódzkiego | pl_PL |
dc.relation.ispartof | Focus on events and narratives in language, psychology, social and medical practice; | |
dc.relation.ispartofseries | Łódzkie Studia z Językoznawstwa Angielskiego i Ogólnego;9 | |
dc.rights | Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 Międzynarodowe | * |
dc.rights.uri | http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ | * |
dc.subject | comprehension | pl_PL |
dc.subject | cultural schemata | pl_PL |
dc.subject | bias | pl_PL |
dc.subject | construal | pl_PL |
dc.subject | narrative | pl_PL |
dc.title | Use of stereotypical social gender roles in an L2 narrative construal task by advanced, Polish learners of English | pl_PL |
dc.type | Book chapter | pl_PL |
dc.page.number | 27-39 | pl_PL |
dc.contributor.authorAffiliation | Uniwersytet Łódzki, Wydział Filologiczny, Katedra Językoznawstwa Angielskiego i Ogólnego | pl_PL |
dc.identifier.eisbn | 978-83-8220-052-2 | |
dc.references | Badio, J. (2020). Narrative as a radial category. Studia Anglica Posnaniensia (to appear in 2020). | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Charniak, E. (1975). Organisation and Inference in Frame-Like Systems of Common-Sense Knowledge. Catalogna: ISCS. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Ervin-Tripp, S. & Küntay, A. (1997). Conversational stories. In T. Givon (Ed.), Conversation: Cognitive, Communicative and Social Perspective, (pp. 136–166). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Evans, V. & Green, M. (2006). Cognitive Linguistics: An Introduction. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Giora, R. (2003). On our Mind: Salience, Context and Figurative Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Labov, W. (1997). Some further steps in narrative analysis. Journal of Narrative and Life History, 7, 395–415. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Labov, W. (2001). Uncovering the event structure of narrative. Georgetown University Round Table. (Georgetown: Georgetown University Press). Available online at: http://www.ling.upenn.edu/~wlabov/uesn.pdf (accessed 9 April 2004). | pl_PL |
dc.references | Labov, W. (2004). Ordinary Events. In C. Fought (Ed.), Sociolinguistic Variation: Critical Reflections, (pp. 31–43). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Labov, W. (2006). Narrative pre-construction. Narrative Inquiry, 16, 37–45. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Labov, W. (2011). Oral narratives of personal experience. In P. C. Hogan (Ed.), The Cambridge Encyclopedia of the Language Sciences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. http://www.ling.upenn.edu/~wlabov/Papers/FebOralNarPE.pdf | pl_PL |
dc.references | Labov, W. & Waletzky, J. (1967). Narrative analysis. In J. Helm (Ed.), Essays on the Verbal and Visual Arts, (pp. 12–44). Seatle: University of Washington Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. (1991). Foundations of Cognitive Grammar, Volume 1 and 2. Stanford: Stanford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Metzing, D. (1979). Frame Conception and Text Understanding. Berlin: de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the Structure of Behaviour. Harvard: Holt, Rinhart and Winston, Inc. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Minsky, M. (1975). A Framework for Representing Knowledge. In P. Winston (Ed.), The Psychology of Computer Visioni, (pp. 211–277). New York: McGraw Hill. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Misztal, M. (1990). Life in Jokes. Warszawa: Wydawnictwa Szkolne i Pedagogiczne. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Schank, R. & Abelson, R. (1977). Scripts, Plans, Goals and Understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Squire, C. (2005). Reading narratives. Group Analysis, 38(1), 91–107. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tversky, B. & Hard, B. M. (2009). Embodied and disembodied cognition: spatial perspetive taking. Cognition, 110, 124–129. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Winograd, T. (1976). Towards A Procedural Analysis of Semantics. Palo Alto: Stanford AI-TR 292. | pl_PL |
dc.identifier.doi | 10.18778/8220-051-5.03 | |