Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorWoodley of Menie, Michael A.
dc.contributor.authorFigueredo, Aurelio José
dc.contributor.authorPeñaherrera-Aguirre, Mateo
dc.contributor.authorJurgenssen, JohnMichael
dc.contributor.authorSarraf, Matthew A.
dc.date.accessioned2022-08-02T12:31:09Z
dc.date.available2022-08-02T12:31:09Z
dc.date.issued2022-07-26
dc.identifier.issn1898-6773
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/11089/42624
dc.description.abstractThe prediction that reduction of negative selection decreases group-level competitiveness, as reflected in increased individual-focused and diminished group-focused moral foundations, is tested. To measure this hypothesized shift in moral foundations, we conduct a culturomic analysis of the utilization frequencies of items sourced from the moral foundations item pool, tracked among Britannic populations from 1800 to 1999 using Google Ngram Viewer. The resultant higher-order factor, which tracks increasing individualizing values and decreasing binding values, is termed Asabiyyah (capturing social cohesion and collective purpose). Two predictors of this factor are examined: change in the strength of intergroup competition and change in levels of indicators of developmental instability. Both the strength of intergroup competition and levels of developmental instability associate with Asabiyyah. Rising developmental instability mediates the impact of inter-group competition, indicating that reduced between-group competition might have relaxed negative selection against mutations, which might reduce Asabiyyah via their effects on inter-genomic transactions. These results must be interpreted carefully, given the clear real-world evidence that explicit commitment to group-oriented values often features in harmful and maladaptive social and political ideologies of an extreme character.en
dc.language.isoen
dc.publisherWydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Łódzkiegopl
dc.relation.ispartofseriesAnthropological Review;2en
dc.rights.urihttps://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0
dc.subjectAsabiyyahen
dc.subjectLexicographic dataen
dc.subjectMoral foundationsen
dc.subjectMulti-level selectionen
dc.titleMoral foundations tracked over 200 years of lexicographic data, and their predictorsen
dc.typeArticle
dc.page.number79-102
dc.contributor.authorAffiliationFigueredo, Aurelio José - University of Arizona, Tucson, Department of Psychology, AZ, USAen
dc.contributor.authorAffiliationPeñaherrera-Aguirre, Mateo - University of Arizona, Tucson, Department of Psychology, AZ, USAen
dc.contributor.authorAffiliationJurgenssen, JohnMichael - Boston University, Boston, Department of Philosophy, MA, USAen
dc.identifier.eissn2083-4594
dc.referencesBachmann SO, Cross E, Kalbassi S, Sarraf MA, Woodley of Menie MA, Baudouin SJ. 2018. Protein pheromone MUP20/Darcin is a vector and target of indirect genetic effects in mice. bioRxiv 1–20.en
dc.referencesBolt J, Inklaar R, de Jong H, van Zanden JL. 2018. Rebasing ‘Maddison’: New income comparisons and the shape of long-run economic development. GGDC Research Memorandum 174.en
dc.referencesBudnik A, Henneberg M. 2017. Worldwide increase of obesity is related to the reduced opportunity for natural selection. PLOS ONE 12:e0170098.en
dc.referencesColleran H. 2016. The cultural evolution of fertility decline. Phil Trans R Soc B 371:20150152.en
dc.referencesCostello TH, Bowes SM, Stevens ST, Waldman ID, Tasimi A, Lilienfeld SO. 2022. Clarifying the structure and nature of left-wing authoritarianism. J Pers Soc Psychol 122:135–170.en
dc.referencesCross ESR. 2019. Investigation of social olfaction in a Neuroligin 3 Knockout mouse model. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, School of Biosciences, Cardiff University, UK.en
dc.referencesCurzan A. 2009. Historical corpus linguistics and evidence of language change. In: A Luedeling and M Kytö, editors. Corpus linguistics. Berlin, Gruyter. 1091–1108.en
dc.referencesDarwin C. 1871. The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. London: John Murray.en
dc.referencesDawkins R. 1981. Selfish genes in race or politics. Nature 289:528.en
dc.referencesDel Giudice M. 2012. The twentieth century reversal of pink-blue gender coding: a scientific urban legend? Arch Sex Behav 41:1321–1323.en
dc.referencesFigueredo AJ, Gorsuch R. 2007. Assortative mating in the Jewel wasp: 2. Sequential canonical analysis as an exploratory form of path analysis. J Ariz-Nev acad sci 39:59–64.en
dc.referencesFigueredo AJ, McKnight PE, McKnight KM, Sidani S. 2000. Multivariate modeling of missing data within and across assessment waves. Addiction 95 Suppl 3:361–380.en
dc.referencesFigueredo AJ, Peñaherrera-Aguirre M, Fernandes HBF, Lomayesva SL, Woodley of Menie MA, Hertler SC, Sarraf MA. 2019a. The Ecology of Empire: The dynamics of strategic differentiation-integration in two competing Western European biocultural groups. PLS 38:210–225.en
dc.referencesFigueredo AJ, Peñaherrera-Aguirre M, Fernandes HBF, Lomayesva SL, Woodley of Menie MA, Herter SC, Sarraf MA. 2019b. War and Peace: A diachronic social biogeography of life history strategy and between-group relations in two Western European populations. J Methods Meas Soc Sci 10:36–75.en
dc.referencesGorsuch RL. 1983. Factor Analysis (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.en
dc.referencesGorsuch RL. 2016. UniMult: For univariate and multivariate data analysis. Altadena, CA: UniMult, Inc. Available at: https://unimult.000webhostapp.com/ [Accessed 30 May 2022].en
dc.referencesGraham J, Haidt J, Nosek BA. 2009. Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. J Pers Soc Psychol 96:1029–1046.en
dc.referencesGreenfield PM. 2013. The changing psychology of culture from 1800 through 2000. Psychol Sci 24:1722–1731.en
dc.referencesGraham J, Nosek BA, Haidt J, Iyer R, Koleva S, Ditto PH. 2011. Mapping the Moral Domain. J Pers Soc Psychol 101:366–385.en
dc.referencesHaidt J. 2007. Moral psychology and the misunderstanding of religion. Edge. Available at: https://www.edge.org/conversation/jonathan_haidt-moral-psychology-and-themisunderstanding-of-religion [Accessed 30 May 2022].en
dc.referencesHaidt J. 2008. Morality. Perspect Psychol Sci 3:65–72.en
dc.referencesHaidt J. 2012. The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion. New York: Vintage Books.en
dc.referencesHaidt J. 2016. Are moral foundations heritable? Probably. The Righteous Mind. Available at: https://righteousmind.com/are-moral-foundations-heritable-probably/#:~:text=Are%20moral%20foundation%20scores%20heritable,heritable%20nor%20stable%20over%20time [Accessed 30 May 2022].en
dc.referencesHamilton WD. 1971. Selection of selfish and altruistic behavior in some extreme models. In: JF Eisenberg, WS Dillon, editors. Man and beast: Comparative social behavior. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington DC, USA, 57–91.en
dc.referencesHertler S, Figueredo AJ, Peñaherrera Aguirre M. 2020. Multilevel Selection: Theoretical Foundations, Historical Examples, and Empirical Evidence. New York, NY: Palgrave Macmillan.en
dc.referencesHills TT, Adelman JS. 2015. Recent evolution of learnability in American English from 1800 to 2000. Cognition 143:87–92.en
dc.referencesFieder M, Huber S. 2018. Political attitude and fertility: Is there a selection for the political extreme? Front Psychol 9:2343.en
dc.referencesIbn Khaldun AA-R. 1377/1969. The Muqaddimah: An Introduction to History. NJ Dawood, editor. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.en
dc.referencesInstitute for Economics and Peace. 2020. Global terrorism index 2020: Measuring the impact of terrorism. National Consortium for the Study of Terrorism and Responses to Terrorism. Available at: https://visionofhumanity.org/wp-content/uploads/2020/11/GTI-2020-web-1.pdfen
dc.referencesKalbassi S, Bachmann SO, Cross E, Roberton VH, Baudouin SJ. 2017. Male and female mice lacking Neuroligin-3 modify the behavior of their wild-type littermates. eNeuro 4:e.0145–17.2017.en
dc.referencesKimmerle EH, Jantz RL. 2006. Secular trends in craniofacial asymmetry studied by geometric morphometry and generalized Procrustes methods. In: DE Slice, editor. Modern morphometrics in physical anthropology. Springer, 247–263.en
dc.referencesKomlos J, Brabec M. 2010. The trend of mean BMI values of US adults, birth cohorts 1882—1986 indicates that the obesity epidemic began earlier than hitherto thought. Am J Hum 22:631–638.en
dc.referencesKondrashov AS. 2017. Crumbling genome: The impact of deleterious mutations on humans. Hoboken: Wiley Blackwell.en
dc.referencesLadle RJ, Correia RA, Do Y, Joo G-J, Malhando ACM, Proulx R, Jepson P. 2016. Conservation culturomics. Front Ecol Environ 14:269–275.en
dc.referencesLinksvayer TA. 2007. Ant species differences determined by epistasis between brood and worker genomes. PLOS ONE 2:e994.en
dc.referencesLynch M. 2016. Mutation and human exceptionalism: our future genetic load. Genetics 202:869–875.en
dc.referencesMarkow TA. 1992. Human handedness and the concept of developmental stability. Genetica 87:87–94.en
dc.referencesMcKnight PE, McKnight KM, Sidani S, Figueredo AJ. 2007. Missing Data: A Gentle Introduction. New York, NY: Guilford Press.en
dc.referencesMcManus IC, Moore J, Freegard M, Rawles R. 2010. Science in the making: right hand, left hand. III: Estimating historical rates of left-handedness. Laterality 15:186–208.en
dc.referencesMichel JB, Shen YK, Aiden AP, Veres A, Gray MK, Aiden EL. 2011. Quantitative analysis of culture using millions of digitized books. Science 331:176–182.en
dc.referencesMoss JT, O’Connor PJ. 2020a. Political correctness and the alt-right: The development of extreme political attitudes. PLOS ONE 15:e0239259.en
dc.referencesMoss JT, O’Connor PJ. 2020b. The Dark Triad traits predict authoritarian political correctness and alt-right attitudes. Heliyon 6:e04453.en
dc.referencesRitz C, Spiess AN. 2008. qpcR: an R package for sigmoidal model selection in quantitative real-time polymerase chain reaction analysis. Bioinformatics 24:1549–1551.en
dc.referencesRohatgi A. 2017. WebPlotDigitizer. Available at: https://apps.automeris.io/wpd/ [Accessed 30 May 2022].en
dc.referencesRoivainen E. 2014. Changes in word usage frequency may hamper comparisons of vocabulary skills: an Ngram analysis of Wordsum, WAIS and WISC test items. J Psychoeduc Assess 32:83–87.en
dc.referencesRoser M, Ritchie H, Ortiz-Ospina E. 2013. World population growth. Available at: https://ourworldindata.org/world-population-growth [Accessed 30 May 2022].en
dc.referencesRühli F, Henneberg M. 2017. Biological future of humankind – Ongoing evolution and the impact of recognition of human biological variation. In: M Tibayrenc and FJ Ayala, edtors. On human nature: Biology, psychology, ethics, politics, and religion. London, UK: Elsevier. 263–275.en
dc.referencesSAS Institute Inc. 2015. Base SAS 9.4 Procedures guide: Statistical Procedures (5th ed.). Cary, NC: SAS Institute Inc. Available at: http://support.sas.com [Accessed 30 May 2022].en
dc.referencesSmith KB, Alford JR, Hibbing JR, Martin NG, Hatemi PK. 2016. Intuitive ethics and political orientations: testing moral foundations as a theory of political ideology. Am J Pol Sci 61:424–437.en
dc.referencesTooby J, Cosmides L. 1992. The psychological foundations of culture. In: J.H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, J. Tooby, editors. The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. Oxford University Press, 19–136.en
dc.referencesVan Valen L. 1962. A study of fluctuating asymmetry. Evolution 16:125–142.en
dc.referencesWoodley of Menie MA, Fernandes HBF. 2016. The secular decline in general intelligence from decreasing developmental stability: theoretical and empirical considerations. Pers Individ Differ 92:194–199.en
dc.referencesWoodley of Menie MA, Fernandes HBF, Figueredo AJ, Meisenberg G. 2015. By their words ye shall know them: Evidence of genic selection against general intelligence and concurrent environmental enrichment in vocabulary usage since the mid 19th century. Front Psychol 6:361.en
dc.referencesWoodley of Menie MA, Figueredo AJ, Sarraf MA. 2019. Slowing life history (K) can account for increasing microinnovation rates and GDP growth, but not macroinnovation rates, which declined following the end of the Industrial Revolution. BBS 42, e213.en
dc.referencesWoodley of Menie MA, Figueredo AJ, Sarraf MA, Hertler S, Fernandes HBF, Peñaherrera Aguirre M. 2017b. The Rhythm of the West: A biohistory of the modern era, AD 1600 to present. J Soc Political Econ Stud. Monograph Series 37. Washington, DC: Council for Social and Economic Studies.en
dc.referencesWoodley of Menie MA, Kanazawa S, Pallesen J, Saraf MA. 2020. Paternal age is negatively associated with religious behavior in a post-60s but not a pre-60s US birth cohort: Evidence for the Social Epistasis Amplification Model. J Relig Health 59:2733–2752.en
dc.referencesWoodley of Menie MA, Saraff M, Pestow R, Fernandes HBF. 2017a. Social epistasis amplifies the fitness costs of deleterious mutations, engendering rapid fitness decline among modernized populations. Evol Psychol Sci 3:181–191.en
dc.referencesYounes N, Ulf-Dietrich R. 2019. Guideline for improving the reliability of Google Ngram studies: Evidence from religious terms. PLOS ONE 14:e0213554.en
dc.contributor.authorEmailWoodley of Menie, Michael A. - Michael.Woodley@vub.be
dc.contributor.authorEmailFigueredo, Aurelio José - ajf@u.arizona.edu
dc.contributor.authorEmailPeñaherrera-Aguirre, Mateo - mpeaher@email.arizona.edu
dc.contributor.authorEmailJurgenssen, JohnMichael - jjurgensen@email.arizona.edu
dc.contributor.authorEmailSarraf, Matthew A. - MS5674@protonmail.com
dc.identifier.doi10.18778/1898-6773.85.2.04
dc.relation.volume85


Files in this item

Thumbnail

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record

https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0
Except where otherwise noted, this item's license is described as https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0