dc.contributor.author | Kowal, Marta | |
dc.date.accessioned | 2024-10-25T06:30:02Z | |
dc.date.available | 2024-10-25T06:30:02Z | |
dc.date.issued | 2024-09-05 | |
dc.identifier.issn | 1898-6773 | |
dc.identifier.uri | http://hdl.handle.net/11089/53516 | |
dc.description.abstract | In recent years, there has been a notable increase in the number of cross-cultural research, marking a positive shift from the predominantly WEIRD (Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic) scientific focus. Most people are not WEIRD, and thus, such a trend is widely appraised. However, cross-cultural research bears many risks, one of which is a language barrier. Conducting studies in various populations that communicate in different languages results in the need to translate the study materials. A proper translation is essential for ensuring the validity and reliability of the data. This study aims to discuss translational practices in cross-cultural research, based on the analysis of studies published between 2017 and 2021 in two respected in cross-cultural social research journals (i.e., Cross-Cultural Research and Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology). The results revealed that one fifth of the analyzed studies lacked crucial information regarding translation procedures. Among the studies that did report on translation methods, back-translation was the most popular approach, with nearly half of the studies utilizing this technique. The recommendations for cross-cultural researchers are outlined, with an emphasis on the sufficient description of the samples, including their nationality and used language. In addition, guidelines for the back-translation are reiterated: 1) forward and 2) back translation, 3) versions’ comparison, 4) pilot study, and 5) revision of the final version. | en |
dc.language.iso | en | |
dc.publisher | Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Łódzkiego | pl |
dc.relation.ispartofseries | Anthropological Review;3 | en |
dc.rights.uri | https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0 | |
dc.subject | forward-back translation | en |
dc.subject | brislin | en |
dc.subject | research methods | en |
dc.subject | linguistic recommendations | en |
dc.title | Translation practices in cross-cultural social research and guidelines for the most popular approach: back-translation | en |
dc.type | Article | |
dc.page.number | 19-32 | |
dc.contributor.authorAffiliation | IDN Being Human Lab – Institute of Psychology, University of Wrocław, Wrocław, Poland | en |
dc.identifier.eissn | 2083-4594 | |
dc.references | Aguinis H, Ramani RS, Alabduljader N. 2018. What you see is what you get? Enhancing methodological transparency in management research. Acad of Manag Annals 12(1): 83–110. https://doi.org/10.5465/annals.2016.0011 | en |
dc.references | Arnett JJ. 2016. The neglected 95%: Why American psychology needs to become less American. 4th edition. Washington: American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/14805-008 | en |
dc.references | ASP. 2020. Awards 2020. Association for Psychological Science. https://www.psychologicalscience.org/2020awards/ [Accessed 10 December 2023]. | en |
dc.references | Bernard M. 2014. What does acting out mean and what can you do to help? – Inside Family Counseling. Inside Family Counseling LLC. http://insidefamilycounseling.com/what-does-acting-out-mean/ | en |
dc.references | Brislin RW. 1970. Back-translation for cross-cultural research. J of Cross-Cult Psych 13:185–216. https://doi.org/10.1177/135910457000100301 | en |
dc.references | Brislin RW. 1983. Cross-Cultural Research in Psychology. Ann Rev of Psych 341:363–400. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.34.020183.002051 | en |
dc.references | Cha ES, Kim KH, Erlen JA. 2007. Translation of scales in cross-cultural research: Issues and techniques. J of Adv Nur 584:386–395. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2648.2007.04242.x | en |
dc.references | Chen MK. 2013. The effect of language on economic behavior: Evidence from savings rates health behaviors and retirement assets. Am Ec Rev 1032:690–731. https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.103.2.690 | en |
dc.references | Choi J, Kushner KE, Mill J, Lai DWL. 2012. Understanding the language the culture and the experience: Translation in cross-cultural research. Int J of Qual Met 115:652–665. https://doi.org/10.1177/160940691201100508 | en |
dc.references | Christensen G, Freese J, Miguel E. 2019. Transparent and reproducible social science research: How to do open science. Berkeley: University of California Press. https://doi.org/10.2307/j.ctvpb3xkg | en |
dc.references | Coles NA, Hamlin JK, Sullivan LL, Parker TH, Altschul D. 2022. Build up big-team science. Nature 6017894:505–507. https://doi.org/10.1038/d41586-022-00150-2 | en |
dc.references | Coughlin CE, Tremblay A. 2013. Proficiency and working memory based explanations for nonnative speakers’ sensitivity to agreement in sentence processing. App Psylin 343:615–646. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716411000890 | en |
dc.references | Croskerry P. 2010. To err is human—And let’s not forget it. CMAJ: Cand Med Assoc J 1825:524. https://doi.org/10.1503/cmaj.100270 | en |
dc.references | Curtarelli M, Van Houten G. 2018. Questionnaire translation in the European Company Survey: Conditions conducive to the effective implementation of a TRAPD-based approach. Inter J of Transl and Interp Res 102:34–54. https://doi.org/10.12807/ti.110202.2018.a04 | en |
dc.references | Douglas SP, Craig CS. 2007. Collaborative and iterative translation: An alternative approach to back translation. J of Intern Mark 151:30–43. https://doi.org/10.1509/jimk.15.1.030 | en |
dc.references | Ennett ST, Tobler NS, Ringwalt CL, Flewelling RL. 1994. How effective is drug abuse resistance education? A meta-analysis of Project DARE outcome evaluations. Amer J of Pub Health 849:1394–1401. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.84.9.1394 | en |
dc.references | Epstein J, Osborne RH, Elsworth GR, Beaton DE, Guillemin F. 2015. Cross-cultural adaptation of the Health Education Impact Questionnaire: Experimental study showed expert committee not back-translation added value. J of Clin Epidm 684:360–369. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2013.07.013 | en |
dc.references | Fink R. 1963. Interviewer training and supervision in a survey of Laos. Intern Soc Sc J 15:21–34. | en |
dc.references | Flake JK, Davidson IJ, Wong O, Pek J. 2022. Construct validity and the validity of replication studies: A systematic review. Amer Psych 774:576–588. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0001006 | en |
dc.references | Gislén A, Warrant EJ, Dacke M, Kröger RHH. 2006. Visual training improves underwater vision in children. Vis Res 4620:3443–3450. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2006.05.004 | en |
dc.references | Hadjichristidis C, Geipel J, Keysar B. 2019. The influence of native language in shaping judgment and choice. In: Srinivasan N, editor. Progress in Brain Research, 253–272. | en |
dc.references | Hadjichristidis C, Geipel J, Surian L. 2017. How foreign language affects decisions: Rethinking the brain-drain model. J of Intern Bus Stud 485:645–651. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41267-016-0040-1 | en |
dc.references | Hagan S, Swartz L, Kilian S, Chiliza B, Bisogno P, Joska J. 2013. The accuracy of interpreting key psychiatric terms by ad hoc interpreters at a South African psychiatric hospital. Afr J of Psych 166. https://doi.org/10.4314/ajpsy.v16i6.54 | en |
dc.references | Henrich J. 2008. A cultural species. In Brown M, editor. Explaining culture scientifically. Seattle: University of Washington Press. 184–210. | en |
dc.references | Henrich J, Heine SJ, Norenzayan A. 2010. The weirdest people in the world? Beh and Br Sci 332–3:61–83. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152X | en |
dc.references | Hudson K. 1978. The jargon of the professions. Berlin: Springer. | en |
dc.references | Hulin CL. 1987. A psychometric theory of evaluations of Item and Scale Translations: Fidelity Across Languages. J of Cross-Cult Psych 182:115–142. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022002187018002001 | en |
dc.references | Klein V, Savaş Ö, Conley TD. 2022. How WEIRD and androcentric is sex research? Global inequities in study populations. J of Sex Res 597:810-817. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2021.1918050 | en |
dc.references | Klotz AC, Swider BW, Kwon SH. 2023. Back-translation practices in organizational research: Avoiding loss in translation. J of Appl Psych 1085:699–727. https://doi.org/10.1037/apl0001050 | en |
dc.references | Kokol P. 2019. Funded and non-funded research literature in software engineering in relation to country determinants. COLLNET J of Scient and Infor Man 131:103–109. https://doi.org/10.1080/09737766.2018.1560637 | en |
dc.references | Kowal M, Sorokowski P, Kulczycki E, Żelaźniewicz A. 2022. The impact of geographical bias when judging scientific studies. Scientomet 127: 265-273. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11192-021-04176-7 | en |
dc.references | Kowal M, Sorokowski P, Dinić BM, Pisanski K, Gjoneska B, et al. 2024. Validation of the Short Version TLS-15 of the Triangular Love Scale TLS-45 across 37 Languages. Arch of Sex Beh 53:839–857. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-023-02702-7 | en |
dc.references | Li Y, Teng W, Tsai L, Lin TMY. 2022. Does English proficiency support the economic development of non-English-speaking countries? The case of Asia. Int J of Educ Dev 92:102623. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijedudev.2022.102623 | en |
dc.references | Lieberoth A, Lin SY, Stöckli S, Han H, Kowal M, Gelpi R, et al. 2021. Stress and worry in the 2020 coronavirus pandemic: Relationships to trust and compliance with preventive measures across 48 countries in the COVIDiSTRESS global survey. Roy Soc Op Sci 82:200589. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.200589 | en |
dc.references | Maneesriwongul W, Dixon JK. 2004. Instrument translation process: A methods review. J of Adv Nurs 482:175–186. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2648.2004.03185.x | en |
dc.references | Milfont TL, Fischer R. 2010. Testing measurement invariance across groups: Applications in cross-cultural research. Intern J of Psych Res 31:Article 1. https://doi.org/10.21500/20112084.857 | en |
dc.references | Morgan E, Zahl B. 2021. Beyond WEIRD: Why we need to make psychology and social science research more inclusive. Templeton world charity foundation. https://live-templeton-next-nhemv.appa.pantheon.site//blog/beyond-weird-why-we-need-make-psychology-and-social-science-research-more-inclusive [Accessed 10 December 2023]. | en |
dc.references | Moshontz H, Campbell L, Ebersole CR, IJzerman H, Urry HL, Forscher PS, et al. 2018. The Psychological Science Accelerator: Advancing Psychology Through a Distributed Collaborative Network. Adv in Meth and Prac in Psych Sci 14:501–515. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245918797607 | en |
dc.references | Pavlenko A. 2012. Affective processing in bilingual speakers: Disembodied cognition? Intern J of Psych 476:405–428. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207594.2012.743665 | en |
dc.references | Pennell BE, Hibben KC, Lyberg LE, Mohler PP, Worku G. 2017. A total survey error perspective on surveys in multinational multiregional and multicultural contexts. In total survey error in practice. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons Ltd. 179–181. | en |
dc.references | Petrosino A, Turpin-Petrosino C, Finckenauer JO. 2000. Well-meaning programs can have harmful effects! Lessons from experiments of programs such as scared straight. Crime & Delinquency 463:354–379. https://doi.org/10.1177/0011128700046003006 | en |
dc.references | Phillips WL. 2019. Cross-cultural differences in visual perception of color illusions depth and pictures. In Cross-Cultural Psychology. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons Ltd. 287–288. | en |
dc.references | Pollet TV, Saxton TK. 2019. How diverse are the samples used in the journals ‘Evolution & Human Behavior’ and ‘Evolutionary Psychology’? Evol Psych Sci 53:357–368. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40806-019-00192-2 | en |
dc.references | Rad MS, Martingano AJ, Ginges J. 2018. Toward a psychology of Homo sapiens: Making psychological science more representative of the human population. Proc Natl Acad Sci 11545:11401–11405. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1721165115 | en |
dc.references | Roberts L, Felser C. 2011. Plausibility and recovery from garden paths in second language sentence processing. Appl Psychlin 322:299–331. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716410000421 | en |
dc.references | Salomone R. 2022. The rise of English: Global politics and the power of language. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | en |
dc.references | Schaffer BS, Riordan CM. 2003. A review of cross-cultural methodologies for organizational research: A best-practices approach. Organ Res Met 62:169–215. https://doi.org/10.1177/1094428103251542 | en |
dc.references | Sinaiko H. 1963. Teleconferencing: Preliminary experiments. Institute for Defense Analyses: Research and Engineering Support Division. Res Paper 108:1–52. | en |
dc.references | Skopec M, Issa H, Reed J, Harris M. 2020. The role of geographic bias in knowledge diffusion: A systematic review and narrative synthesis. Res Int and Peer Rev 51:2. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41073-019-0088-0 | en |
dc.references | Sorokowski P, Kowal M, Sternberg RJ, Aavik T, Akello G, Alhabahba MM, et al. 2023. Modernization collectivism and gender equality predict love experiences in 45 countries. Sci Rep 131 Article 1. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-26663-4 | en |
dc.references | Spector PE, Liu C, Sanchez JI. 2015. Methodological and substantive issues in conducting multinational and cross-cultural research. Ann Rev of Organ Psych and Organ Beh 21:101–131. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-032414-111310 | en |
dc.references | Sternberg RJ. 1986. A triangular theory of love. Psych Rev 93:119–135. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.93.2.119 | en |
dc.references | Survey Research Center. 2016. Guidelines for best practice in cross-cultural Surveys. Survey Research Center Institute for Social Research University of Michigan. http://www.ccsg.isr.umich.edu/ [Accessed 10 December 2023]. | en |
dc.references | Thalmayer AG, Toscanelli C, Arnett JJ. 2021. The neglected 95% revisited: Is American psychology becoming less American? Amer Psych 76:116–129. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000622 | en |
dc.references | Tindle R. 2021. Improving the global reach of psychological research. Disc Psych 11:5. https://doi.org/10.1007/s44202-021-00004-4 | en |
dc.references | Tyupa S. 2023. A theoretical framework for back-translation as a quality assessment tool. New Voices in Transl Stud 71:35–46. https://doi.org/10.14456/nvts.2011.4 | en |
dc.references | Valdez D, Montenegro MS, Crawford BL, Turner RC, Lo WJ, Jozkowski KN. 2021. Translation frameworks and questionnaire design approaches as a component of health research and practice: A discussion and taxonomy of popular translation frameworks and questionnaire design approaches. Soc Sci & Med 278:113931. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2021.113931 | en |
dc.references | van de Vijver FJR, Leung K. 2011. Equivalence and bias: A review of concepts models and data analytic procedures. In: Cross-cultural research methods in psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 17–45 | en |
dc.references | Vandevoorde L, Daems J, Defrancq B. 2019. New Empirical Perspectives on Translation and Interpreting. Abingdon-on-Thames: Routledge. | en |
dc.references | Vujcich D, Roberts M, Gu Z, Kao SC, Lobo R, Mao L, et al. 2021. Translating best practice into real practice: Methods results and lessons from a project to translate an English sexual health survey into four Asian languages. PloS one 1612:e0261074. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0261074 | en |
dc.references | Weiner IB, Reynolds WM, Miller GE. 2012. Handbook of psychology educational psychology. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons. | en |
dc.references | Werner O, Campbell D. 1969. Translating working through interpreters and the problem of decentering. In Naroll R, editor. A handbook of cultural anthropology. New York: American Museum of Natural History. | en |
dc.contributor.authorEmail | marta7kowal@gmail.com | |
dc.identifier.doi | 10.18778/1898-6773.87.3.02 | |
dc.relation.volume | 87 | |